Skip to main content

A matter of semantics: same concepts, different divisions


Eighteen containers in assorted shapes and sizes on display in the corner of the room.





Fourteen EFL teachers organized in small groups according to
their L1: English, Russian, Hebrew, Arabic, French.





Aim: categorise the objects; discussion in the group should
be held in your L1





Purpose: to show that the same objects
will fall into different categories depending on the language you use to
categorise them. 







For example, the English-speaking group divided the objects
which you can see in the picture into 8 categories: bottle (4), container (5),
tube (2), can (2), jar (1), case (1), jug (1), case (1), canister (1). A
British English speaker further divided cans into a can (soft drink) and a tin
(of tuna). Hebrew speakers came up with 7 categories, with the can/tin of tuna ending up in the same
category as “English” containers (including tupperware) while Russian speakers
grouped the notorious can/tin together with the jar under the same title
“banka”.





Idea: not mine. It was inspired by a study conducted by Malt,
Sloman and Gennari (2003) who tried to understand the relations between semantic categories among speakers of 3 languages: (American) English, (Argentine) Spanish and
Mandarin Chinese. Only in their study they used 60 containers (I couldn’t carry so
many from home). After sorting the objects into piles, the participants of the
study were asked to name the objects (in their native language). The English
speakers had a total of 7 names, i.e. all the objects were divided into 7
categories, while the Chinese group had only 5 categories. Interestingly, the
Spanish speaking group ended up with 14 different names – some of their
categories with only one or two objects in them.





The most interesting thing about the experiment, however, is the degree of variation in
distribution of names. For example, what the Spanish speakers described as
“frasco” or “frasquito” (diminutive form) consisted of 28 items; most of these
corresponded to what English speakers called “jar” (19 items), but also included
what English speakers referred to as “bottle” (6 items) and “container” (3
items). Another group of six items Spanish speakers described as “bidon” was
comprised of 3 “English” jugs, 2 containers and 1 bottle. Other bottles
(English speakers identified a total of 16) ended up with different Spanish labels, for
example, botella (3 items), mamadera (2 items) and envase (2 items). Curiously, this last, "envase” group also included 4 objects described by English speakers
as “containers” and other sundry objects.





The comparison between Chinese and English showed the same
pattern. Unlike their Spanish counterparts, though, Chinese speakers had one large
group of 40 items called “Ping2a ” which encompassed all jars (19) as well as
13 bottles and 8 containers. Another 3 containers were placed with 2 bottles
and 5 cans into one category Chinese speakers labelled as “Guan4”.







What does it all have to do with English language teaching?





The study is published in the fascinating volume entitled Language in Mind which presents a modern take on - and perhaps an attempt to revive - the (weak) version of the theory of
linguistic relativity. Also known as the Sapir–Whorf Hypothesis, the theory faced
harsh criticism and was considered dead for most of the second part of the 20th
century. According to the theory, the structure of one’s language influences one’s
understanding of the world. In other words, language affects thought. 





Although Malt
et al’s study doesn’t actually prove that language affects thought, it clearly demonstrates how
different languages divide the world differently. And that’s what I wanted to
show in my experiment: how because of carving up reality differently words
rarely have an exact equivalent in another language. 





Even for concrete nouns, such as the ones describing the containers, semantic fields of L1 and English
do not necessarily overlap. And if different languages do not “agree” on what
categories different items should be assigned to, distinctions between how more
abstract notions are expressed verbally across languages become even fuzzier. For
example, English “convenient” and “comfortable” correspond roughly to the same
word in Russian (“udobniy”), thus often leading to confusion and, inevitably, wrong word choices among Russian learners of English (*The sofa is convenient). The phenomenon when a word in a language has two (or more) equivalents in another language is known as "semantic split". Or take an example from Turkish: both "scenery”
and “view” are rendered into Turkish as “manzara”. Semantic partitioning of the world may also be different in different language due to cultural or historical reasons. For example, many languages do not distinguish between “to blame” and “to accuse” or “to be accused” and “to be guilty”. 





Word meanings are a messy area. The lack of direct correspondence between L1 words and L2
words is something beginner learners often find difficult to come to terms with. But we,
language teachers, should disabuse them of the notion that an English word = (is equal to) an L1 word relatively early on. As much as L1 provides
initial support when clarifying meaning of a new word, it is also important to
get across to learners how the English word may behave differently from its L1
equivalent in relation to the meaning it denotes and other words it is associated with, i.e. co-text.








  • Do you have examples of similar messy relationships between certain words in English and your students’ L1?






  • Do you do anything in class to draw your students’ attention to the fact
    that semantic fields of two words – English and its L1 equivalent – rarely
    overlap 100%?







References



Malt, B.C., Sloman, S.A. & Gennari, S. (2003). Speaking vs. thinking about objects and actions. In D. Genter & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.) Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 81-111). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.











Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Benefits Of Healthy eating Turmeric every day for the body

One teaspoon of turmeric a day to prevent inflammation, accumulation of toxins, pain, and the outbreak of cancer.  Yes, turmeric has been known since 2.5 centuries ago in India, as a plant anti-inflammatory / inflammatory, anti-bacterial, and also have a good detox properties, now proven to prevent Alzheimer's disease and cancer. Turmeric prevents inflammation:  For people who

Women and children overboard

It's the  Catch-22  of clinical trials: to protect pregnant women and children from the risks of untested drugs....we don't test drugs adequately for them. In the last few decades , we've been more concerned about the harms of research than of inadequately tested treatments for everyone, in fact. But for "vulnerable populations,"  like pregnant women and children, the default was to exclude them. And just in case any women might be, or might become, pregnant, it was often easier just to exclude us all from trials. It got so bad, that by the late 1990s, the FDA realized regulations and more for pregnant women - and women generally - had to change. The NIH (National Institutes of Health) took action too. And so few drugs had enough safety and efficacy information for children that, even in official circles, children were being called "therapeutic orphans."  Action began on that, too. There is still a long way to go. But this month there was a sign that

Not a word was spoken (but many were learned)

Video is often used in the EFL classroom for listening comprehension activities, facilitating discussions and, of course, language work. But how can you exploit silent films without any language in them? Since developing learners' linguistic resources should be our primary goal (well, at least the blogger behind the blog thinks so), here are four suggestions on how language (grammar and vocabulary) can be generated from silent clips. Split-viewing Split-viewing is an information gap activity where the class is split into groups with one group facing the screen and the other with their back to the screen. The ones facing the screen than report on what they have seen - this can be done WHILE as well as AFTER they watch. Alternatively, students who are not watching (the ones sitting with their backs to the screen) can be send out of the classroom and come up with a list of the questions to ask the 'watching group'. This works particularly well with action or crime scenes with