Skip to main content

Closely connected






Photo by Sudhamshu Hebbar on Flickr [CC BY 2.0]


An article written by the British linguist Vyvyan Evans
entitled “Language
Instinct is a Myth”
which I shared on Twitter the other day triggered a lively discussion with my colleagues. One of the questions raised on Twitter was how come the idea that we are born with a built-in language capacity
(aka the innateness hypothesis) has prevailed for so long and Chomsky, its main
promoter, is part of all Master's in TESOL programmes if the theory has largely
been discredited (Scott Thornbury asks the same question on his in X
is for X-bar Theory
).







This was indeed the case on my MA programme: Behaviorism and Chomsky
took up a large part of two of my Psycholinguistics courses while such a fascinating,
more recent theory as Connectionism received scant or almost no attention. Now
that I am on the giving end, i.e. giving rather than listening to lectures, I
was also surprised at the lack of references to Connectionism in an SLA course syllabus which I inherited (and revamped completely). Chomsky, on the
other hand, is such an enduring staple of TEFL/TESOL courses that even BA students come
to the course already knowing him – or at least having heard his name.





For an overview of Chomsky's theory see THIS POST by Geoff Jordan or THIS POST by Kylie Barker.





Connectionism is also conspicuously absent from Vyvyan
Evan’s criticism of Chomsky although it provides much a stronger argument
against the innateness hypothesis. What is Connectionism and how is it connected
to this blog?







It doesn’t sound right but I can’t explain why…





The word “grammar” usually conjures up in the learner’s –
and teacher’s – mind images of verb tables and rules which can be memorized and
tested. Indeed, some rules – or rather rules of thumb – exist and can be called
upon when punctuating a sentence (add an apostrophe after plural nouns ending
with –s) or spelling a word (“i before e except after c”). Learners can be
given useful rules such as: We add –s/-es to make the plural form or most nouns
or add –d/-ed to make the past tense of regular verbs. The rules that can be
formulated and verbalized are known as explicit rules. A greater number of
linguistic rules though – probably much greater than explicit rules – are implicit
rules, i.e. rules which competent language users (not only native speakers!)
know but cannot express verbally. The knowledge of implicit rules is evident
when we can tell if a sentence is correct or incorrect just because it sounds
right. This intuitive feel for grammaticality, however, has nothing to do with
the sixth sense; it has sound psychological foundations.





Artificial neural networks (explained as simply as possible)







Via Wikimedia Commons [CC BY-SA 3.0]


The human brain is made up of a massive number of neurons
meshed into complex networks. In order to understand how information is stored
across neural networks of the brain, artificial neural networks can be created on
the computer. Artificial neural networks simulate how the brain functions when processing information and consist of many units (representing neurons) and their connections
(representing synapses), hence the name “connectionism”.




Already in the 1980s, connectionist experiments shed considerable
light on the processes underlying human cognition and, specifically, language
acquisition. For example, a computer simulation created by Rumelhart and McClelland
(1986) was “trained” to predict irregular past forms of the verbs it had not
previously encountered. For example, after the network “learned” that found is
the past form of find, and bound is the past of bind, the network would produce wound in response to wind.












Via Wikimedia Commons [CC BY 1.0]


Now on to something truly phenomenal. Neural networks have
been shown to exhibit the same “faulty” behaviour as humans. The next set of
verbs which was fed into Rumelhart and McClelland’s network consisted of both irregular
and regular (-ed) verbs. As the number of regular verbs began to grow, the
network started to produce errors such as *broked instead of broke or *taked
instead of took. But after more training, regular and irregular verbs fell into
place as the network recovered and started producing correct forms again.







As you probably know, this process is very similar to the
developmental stages language acquirers – mainly native speakers but also L2
learners – go through. Initially they produce correct irregular forms, such as went and took,
but, as encounters with regular verbs become more and more frequent (washed, cleaned, started,
finished
), they backslide to *goed and *taked. Chomskyans would, of course,
explain this as learners starting to grapple with the rules of the past simple
and overgeneralising them to irregular verbs. But Rumelhart and McClelland’s neural
network displayed the same psycholinguistic phenomena as overgeneralization and
backsliding – in the absence of any grammar rules! 





Rules or connections?





Connectionists offer a simple but compelling explanation of
this phenomenon: the neural network learned the pattern on which the past tense
is formed as a result of exposure to linguistic data. Regular verbs showed such a strongly
consistent pattern in the input that the connections formed between units in response to regular
verbs outweighed all the connections activated by irregular verbs.





Posing a radical challenge to the classical rule view of
language, connectionism posits that language learning has nothing to do with
learning rules, although language behaviour may ultimately appear to be rule-governed.
Just like the artificial network was not taught the rules of the past simple,
mental representations of rules need not be present in explicit form anywhere
in the brain. According to connectionism, language acquirers merely form mental
associations between various elements (phonemes, morphemes, words etc) which
frequently occur together in language input.





Connectionism and lexical chunking



You are probably wondering what it all has to do with this
blog. In fact, connectionism and its younger brother “emergentism” are compatible with the idea of lexical chunking and provide a solid psycholinguistic
base for the Lexical approach.







  • Chunks consist of words that often go together:  of course, I hope so, to make things worse,
    it’s been a long time since…,
    i.e. they are frequently co-occurring elements in language 

  • Learning a language is not a matter of mastering explicit
    grammar rules as I have argued HERE

  • Learning chunks can help establish important patterns which pave the way to grammar acquisition - see HERE

  • Repetition plays a crucial role – learners
    need multiple exposures to “strengthen the connections” between co-occurring
    elements

  • Although explicit teaching of grammar rules does not result in
    implicit knowledge it may speed up the process of grammar acquisition through noticing







So, to return to the discussion with my fellow professionals
on Twitter: should Chomsky be banished from TESOL programmes’ syllabi?
Certainly not. As a starting point in SLA research it merits analysis and discussion.
Without understanding Chomsky’s Universal Grammar hypothesis, it would be hard,
for example, to explain Krashen* or to grasp the significance of the nature vs. nurture
debate as regards language acquisition. But the question remains why Chomsky's view has dominated the field for so long. Is it because, as Vyvyan Evans claims, it's simple?



What do you think?





References


Rumelhart, D. E.,& McClelland, J. L. (1986).On learning the past tense of English verbs. In J. L. McClelland,
D. E. Rumelhart & the PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure
of cognition, Vol. 2: Psychological and biological models
(pp. 216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.





_____________________


* Interestingly - and perhaps ironically - Michael Lewis draws heavily on the work of Krashen, who is clearly a Chomskyan






Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Benefits Of Healthy eating Turmeric every day for the body

One teaspoon of turmeric a day to prevent inflammation, accumulation of toxins, pain, and the outbreak of cancer.  Yes, turmeric has been known since 2.5 centuries ago in India, as a plant anti-inflammatory / inflammatory, anti-bacterial, and also have a good detox properties, now proven to prevent Alzheimer's disease and cancer. Turmeric prevents inflammation:  For people who

Women and children overboard

It's the  Catch-22  of clinical trials: to protect pregnant women and children from the risks of untested drugs....we don't test drugs adequately for them. In the last few decades , we've been more concerned about the harms of research than of inadequately tested treatments for everyone, in fact. But for "vulnerable populations,"  like pregnant women and children, the default was to exclude them. And just in case any women might be, or might become, pregnant, it was often easier just to exclude us all from trials. It got so bad, that by the late 1990s, the FDA realized regulations and more for pregnant women - and women generally - had to change. The NIH (National Institutes of Health) took action too. And so few drugs had enough safety and efficacy information for children that, even in official circles, children were being called "therapeutic orphans."  Action began on that, too. There is still a long way to go. But this month there was a sign that

Not a word was spoken (but many were learned)

Video is often used in the EFL classroom for listening comprehension activities, facilitating discussions and, of course, language work. But how can you exploit silent films without any language in them? Since developing learners' linguistic resources should be our primary goal (well, at least the blogger behind the blog thinks so), here are four suggestions on how language (grammar and vocabulary) can be generated from silent clips. Split-viewing Split-viewing is an information gap activity where the class is split into groups with one group facing the screen and the other with their back to the screen. The ones facing the screen than report on what they have seen - this can be done WHILE as well as AFTER they watch. Alternatively, students who are not watching (the ones sitting with their backs to the screen) can be send out of the classroom and come up with a list of the questions to ask the 'watching group'. This works particularly well with action or crime scenes with