Skip to main content

Teaching vocabulary out of context: conclusions

This follows on my earlier post Teaching vocabulary out of context: is it worth the time?





About a month ago I blogged about my mini action research on decontextualised vocabulary learning. The post  generated some discussion with some people arguing that there was nothing decontexualised about it - you can read the original post and the comments here. The main finding was that on the post-test there was no difference between the items which were learnt out of context and the items presented in class in context. So is decontextualised vocabulary teaching a justified strategy?





Obvious benefits



Even though the decontextualised “Go home and look up” activity
produced mixed results and there was a lot of reteaching involved, I still consider it to be successful. Besides enriching
their vocabulary it gives learners a sense of autonomy as well as
responsibility for their learning, not to mention practice with using
dictionaries, an often overlooked skill in ELT. I will definitely keep giving my students such tasks it in
the future.



The fact there was
no significant difference between the explicitly taught and independently
learnt items on the test attests to the fact that an initial encounter (whether
contextualised or decontextualised) is not as important as subsequent elaboration
on various aspects of the word's meaning, repeated encounters with the item, rehearsal and
recycling. The research seems to corroborate it (see for example Hulstijn 2001).



Ifs, buts and qualifications



Decontextualised vocabulary learning was frowned upon in the
communicative teaching tradition.However, recent years have seen its resurgence due to the emerging
evidence that this type of learning is legitimate for studying the basic vocabulary
quickly (see Laufer 2009). Note that Laufer uses the word “basic”. In other words, in
order to get to some threshold level, learners need to quickly acquire a large
number of words, and decontextualised learning is a perfectly justified strategy. But when it comes to post-intermediate learners, I would only use decontextualised learning of discrete items if learners:






1)      are trained to look up words in a monolingual dictionary, carefully studying the examples provided and patterns of use;


2)      have a subsequent opportunity to contextualise with the teacher providing guidance and corrective feedback;


3)      are given ample opportunities for recall and recycling (as with any vocabulary learning)





Another issue which has been recently raised in the literature on vocabulary acquisition is the question of effectiveness vs. efficiency of a vocabulary task, i.e. how effective a vocabulary task is in terms of the time spent on it. Coming from this perspective, independent decontextualised learning for post-intermediate learners does not seem to be time efficient because a lot of time has to be spent on remedying learners’ problems and re-teaching mislearnt items



References






  • Hulstijn,
    J. H. (2001). Intentional and incidental second language vocabulary
    learning: A reappraisal of elaboration, rehearsal and automaticity. In Robinson,
    P. (ed.), Cognition and second language instruction. New York: Cambridge
    University
    Press,
    258−286.



  • Laufer, B (2009). Research timeline: Second
    language vocabulary acquisition from language input and from form-focused
    activities. In Language Teaching, 42(3), 341-354.












Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Benefits Of Healthy eating Turmeric every day for the body

One teaspoon of turmeric a day to prevent inflammation, accumulation of toxins, pain, and the outbreak of cancer.  Yes, turmeric has been known since 2.5 centuries ago in India, as a plant anti-inflammatory / inflammatory, anti-bacterial, and also have a good detox properties, now proven to prevent Alzheimer's disease and cancer. Turmeric prevents inflammation:  For people who

Women and children overboard

It's the  Catch-22  of clinical trials: to protect pregnant women and children from the risks of untested drugs....we don't test drugs adequately for them. In the last few decades , we've been more concerned about the harms of research than of inadequately tested treatments for everyone, in fact. But for "vulnerable populations,"  like pregnant women and children, the default was to exclude them. And just in case any women might be, or might become, pregnant, it was often easier just to exclude us all from trials. It got so bad, that by the late 1990s, the FDA realized regulations and more for pregnant women - and women generally - had to change. The NIH (National Institutes of Health) took action too. And so few drugs had enough safety and efficacy information for children that, even in official circles, children were being called "therapeutic orphans."  Action began on that, too. There is still a long way to go. But this month there was a sign that

Not a word was spoken (but many were learned)

Video is often used in the EFL classroom for listening comprehension activities, facilitating discussions and, of course, language work. But how can you exploit silent films without any language in them? Since developing learners' linguistic resources should be our primary goal (well, at least the blogger behind the blog thinks so), here are four suggestions on how language (grammar and vocabulary) can be generated from silent clips. Split-viewing Split-viewing is an information gap activity where the class is split into groups with one group facing the screen and the other with their back to the screen. The ones facing the screen than report on what they have seen - this can be done WHILE as well as AFTER they watch. Alternatively, students who are not watching (the ones sitting with their backs to the screen) can be send out of the classroom and come up with a list of the questions to ask the 'watching group'. This works particularly well with action or crime scenes with